This is a review of a recently-published book, Bird Sense by Tim Birkhead. it's definitely a book I want for Christmas....
Who’d be a bird anyway? Chickens have bi-focal
vision: one eye for the close-up work of pecking seed; one for the
fox on the horizon or the hawk
in the sky. Peregrine falcons don’t swoop directly on prey – as
the crow flies, to coin a phrase – but in a wide arc, using the
right eye. Mallard ducks on the ground and swifts on the wing both
nod off with half the brain at work and one eye wide open watching
for danger.
Bird Sense: What it’s Like to be a Bird, by Tim
Birkhead
Nightingales in Berlin have to up their vocal
performance by 14 decibels to be heard over the traffic; great tits
in the city keep down the volume but change the pitch or the
frequency to get the message across. The oilbird of Ecuador sleeps
with its eyes closed but then it could even fly with its eyes closed:
like a bat, it uses echolocation to work out where it is in total
darkness.
The ears of the great grey owl are asymmetrical –
higher on one side than the other – the better to pinpoint prey on
the vertical as well as the horizontal axis. That is why it can swoop
on a mouse under the snow. All listeners can localise a source of
sound by unconsciously measuring the difference in time as the waves
arrive at each separate ear: for small birds, this would dwindle to
less than a millionth of a second so little birds move their heads
from side to side to increase the range.
Avian hearing ability varies according to the
season. So do other features. In winter, the testes of the cock
sparrow dwindle to the size of a pinhead; with the nesting season,
they swell to the volume of a baked bean. The ability to sing tends
to surge with the urge to nest. A hormonal response that varies with
daylight length also does strange things to the brain: the ability to
acquire and deliver song dwindles, and the relevant area of the bird
brain shrinks, in the winter. This is, says Tim Birkhead in Bird
Sense, “a sensible energy-saving tactic” because brains are a big
expense: the human brain for instance uses 10 times as much energy as
any other organ.
Books like this often deliver far more value than
their titles might suggest. Bipedal mammals with limited vision and
hearing that fades with age and too much rock music can’t really
“know” what it is like to be a penguin,
a flamingo, or a tropical hummingbird,
but they can marvel at the difference. So the continuous reference to
a human template also provides a lot of useful general instruction in
physiology, anatomy and evolution. The simple attempts to address
each question of bird sense – taste, smell and touch as well as
hearing and vision – involves science history, and not just, for
instance, the first scholarly papers on echo location in barn owls.
Aristotle is there (he usually is) and Cuvier,
Buffon, Humboldt, Darwin, and Audubon. Bartolomaeus Eustachius of the
Eustachian tubes is there, as he would be in a book that addresses
hearing; along with Gabriel Fallopius who identified the
semi-circular canal in the human middle ear (along, of course, with
an important feature of the female reproductive system). So,
unexpectedly, is US president Calvin Coolidge, whose name is invoked
in the last page of the notes to label a well-known sexual
phenomenon: that lustful enthusiasm in a male tends to increase with
novel females.
Touchingly, given the importance, urgency and
frequency of sex in bird life, it becomes quite difficult to answer
the big question: do birds enjoy lovemaking? The European dunnock
is quick on the job – it took high-speed photography to establish
that the act was over in one tenth of a second. The great vasa parrot
of Madagascar
is locked in genital embrace for an hour and a half, seemingly
whispering sweet nothings. But, says Birkhead, the only instance in
which sexual pleasure was strikingly apparent (and he describes a lot
of field work in this book) involved an African bird called the
red-billed buffalo weaver. The starling-sized male has a somewhat
puzzling false penis and was observed “after 30 minutes of vigorous
venery” to experience an apparent orgasm.
“This was unheard of: no other bird in the world
was known to climax. In a state of high excitement, Mark phoned from
Germany to tell me.” Quite how Birkhead’s fellow-researcher Mark
confirmed this epochal discovery is an indelicate story, but the
phrase “at first hand” would describe it fairly.
The chapter on avian emotions – including the
search for evidence of romantic love in apparently monogamous species
– is navigated (yes, there’s a chapter on navigation too) with
caution and Birkhead delicately adds “I am not going to speculate
about the emotions that might be involved in avian infidelity”.
This book kicked off as three-to-one favourite to win the Royal
Society Winton Prize. I can see why.
No comments:
Post a Comment